, 2005) But finding individual neurons that respond particularly

, 2005). But finding individual neurons that respond particularly well to a moving fly is only part of the story: the fact that most neurons respond to a range of visual stimuli immediately tells us that the representation is more complex. An alternative view is that the important features

of a stimulus are represented by a “distributed code” in which information is contained in the pattern of activity across a population of neurons. In this second view, to really understand what the “frog’s eye tells the frog’s brain,” we must record the activity of all the neurons providing the retinal output. This is a formidable technical challenge: how do we sample activity across a complete population of Ruxolitinib ic50 sensory Ulixertinib concentration neurons? Markus Meister provided

the first approach by placing the retina of a salamander on an array of electrodes that recorded spikes from hundreds of ganglion cells simultaneously (Meister et al., 1995). In this issue of Neuron, Nikolaou et al. (2012) use imaging to achieve a similar goal, mapping the visual signal projected from the retina to the optic tectum of zebrafish. The optic tectum receives the major part of the retinal output—it is one of the largest parts of the brain by volume and analogous to the superior colliculus in mammals. In zebrafish, as in frogs, the tectum processes visual signals that drive motor outputs, contributing to behaviors such as avoidance of objects and predators as well as capture of prey (Nevin et al., 2010).

Although there may be “fly detectors” in the tectum, it clearly plays a more general role in directing the animal’s movements relative to its environment. Purely heuristic approaches will not, therefore, provide a proper understanding of the function of this part of the brain; we need to build a more complete and systematic picture of the information Rebamipide transmitted to the tectum and how this information is distributed—a “functional map” (Figure 1). To begin this mapping exercise, Nikolaou et al. (2012) made transgenic zebrafish expressing SyGCaMP3, a fluorescent protein that reports the activation of synapses by sensing the presynaptic calcium signal driving vesicle fusion. SyGCaMPs are a fusion of a genetically encoded calcium indicator of the GCaMP family to synaptophysin, a protein in the membrane of synaptic vesicles (Dreosti et al., 2009). By use of a promoter specific for retinal ganglion cells, Nikolaou et al. (2012) targeted SyGCaMP3 to all the axon terminals transmitting visual signals to the tectum. This approach is similar to one in which SyGCaMP2 was used to image the preceding stage of transmission of the visual signal, from bipolar cells to ganglion cells (Odermatt et al., 2012).

, 2011), as is seen in PD brain pathology In the homozygous stat

, 2011), as is seen in PD brain pathology. In the homozygous state, GBA mutations are associated with Gaucher’s disease, with severe lysosomal dysfunction typically early in life (Mazzulli et al., 2011). As expected, iPSC derived neurons that harbor triplication of the αSyn locus display similarly increased accumulation of αSyn protein ( Byers et al.,

2011). Regulation of αSyn gene expression is species specific, and appears to be modified both in familial and sporadic PD brain ( Rhinn et al., 2012); thus in vitro human neuronal models may prove to be particularly useful. ALS is characterized by a progressive loss of motor neurons in the spinal cord, leading to difficulty with movement and breathing. Rare Osimertinib Selleckchem MLN0128 familial forms of ALS have been unambiguously associated with mutations in superoxide dismutase-1 (SOD1), transactive response DNA-binding 43 (TDP-43), fused in sarcoma (FUS), C9orf72, and approximately

a dozen other genes (Ferraiuolo et al., 2011). A common theme in the context of several of these familial forms—including mutant forms of SOD1, TDP-43, FUS, and C9orf72—is the formation of cytoplasmic aggregates (Ash et al., 2013, Da Cruz and Cleveland, 2011 and Mori et al., 2013). Furthermore, TDP-43 aggregates are found in the majority of nonfamilial “sporadic” ALS cases even in the absence of known mutations, supporting PD184352 (CI-1040) the idea that common mechanisms underlie the familial and sporadic forms. Cytoplasmic TDP-43 aggregates are typically seen in neurons and astrocytes along with concurrent “clearing” of the normal nuclear localization of TDP-43, and this has opened the possibility that loss of nuclear TDP-43 function, as well as aggregation, may play a role in pathology. Model organism studies, from mice to yeast, have brought significant insight into the role of genes such as TDP-43 in vivo, but questions persist about the specific mechanism of action in the context of human motor neurons. For instance, the relative importance of protein aggregates, nuclear

clearing, and the nonautonomous impact of astroglial pathology on motor neuron loss ( Da Cruz and Cleveland, 2011) is unclear. Initial analyses of iPSC-derived spinal motor neurons with mutations in TDP-43 have reported evidence of reduced motor neuron survival in vitro, particularly in the context of an oxidative toxin, arsenite, and accumulation of TDP-43 ( Bilican et al., 2012 and Egawa et al., 2012). A critical point is that these studies did not include validation using a “rescue” approach or cohorts of sufficient size for a statistical analysis, both of which are essential. A cohort of iPSC-derived motor neuron cultures that harbor ALS-associated mutations in SOD1 has been generated ( Boulting et al.

The purpose of the study and the procedures were explained and si

The purpose of the study and the procedures were explained and signed informed consent was obtained from the parents or legal guardians. Enrolled children were randomized to receive three or five doses at six, 10 and

14 weeks or at six, 10, 14, 18 and 22 weeks respectively, along with scheduled childhood vaccines, based on randomization codes provided by a biostatistician not connected with the study as serially numbered opaque sealed envelopes. All routine vaccines were administered as per the National Immunization Schedule or the Indian Academy of Paediatrics Schedule at six-10-14 weeks of age (i.e., DTPw/DTap, Haemophilus influenza type b, OPV/IPV and, Hepatitis B) [21], followed

by the Rotarix vaccination at six, 10 and 14 weeks, and in the five dose arm two additional doses at 18 and 22 weeks. Two blood samples check details of 3.5 ml were collected from all infants, the first prior to the administration of the first dose of Rotarix vaccine and the second 28 days after the last (third or fifth) dose of vaccine administration. Each sample was analyzed for rotavirus specific IgA by an antibody-sandwich enzyme immunoassay which has been validated by the same laboratory that carried out pre-licensure vaccine evaluations for several vaccines [22]. Briefly, 96 well microtiter plates were coated TCL overnight with serum from rabbits hyper-immunized with purified double-layered SA11 derived rotavirus particles. KRX-0401 order The next day, partially purified cell culture lysates derived from G1P8 (RIX4414) infected or mock-infected MA 104 cells were added. Dilutions of a standard pool of human serum assigned an arbitrary value of 1000 U or test sera were added followed by the addition of biotinylated rabbit anti-human IgA, peroxidase-conjugated avidin-biotin, and substrate (orthophenylenediamine/H2O2).

After 30 min, the reaction was stopped with 1 M H2SO4, and absorbance was read at 492 nm. The IgA titer was determined by comparing the optical density values from sample wells with the standard curve based on derived units of IgA arbitrarily assigned to a pooled human serum samples, as previously described [22]. Seropositivity was defined as an anti-rotavirus IgA concentration ≥20 U/ml. Seroconversion was considered as the presence of ≥20 U/ml anti-rotavirus IgA in infants who were negative for anti-RV IgA prior to vaccination. A cut-off of 20 RV-IgA units [11], or at least twofold changes in case of a higher baseline values. Seroconversion rate and geometric mean concentrations (GMCs) were assessed at one month after dose three or dose five of Rotarix administration.

, 2011) However, subsequent work using recombinant synuclein has

, 2011). However, subsequent work using recombinant synuclein has confirmed that even the nondenatured recombinant protein is intrinsically disordered and loses its α-helical conformation after dissociation from membranes (Fauvet et al., 2012). Loss of helicity Tanespimycin could thus reflect the dilution inherent in preparing an

extract, with the helical state maintained at higher concentrations (Dettmer et al., 2013 and Wang et al., 2011), but NMR studies in E. coli have in fact suggested that macromolecular crowding maintains the disordered state of synuclein ( McNulty et al., 2006). It is also possible that synuclein folds to form a multimer only in mammalian cells, but the analysis of native brain synuclein has recently confirmed its almost entirely monomeric state ( Burré et al., 2013). Recently, it has also been shown that

synuclein can assemble into an oligomer (possibly tetramer) on nanoparticles ( Varkey et al., 2013), but this phenomenon seems to differ from the ability of a preformed tetramer to interact with membranes ( Wang et al., 2011). At this point, it remains possible that α-synuclein adopts a helical tetrameric state in solution, but the evidence is not definitive. The unavoidable dilution that accompanies purification of native synuclein complicates the analysis, but it is perhaps more important to acknowledge that despite extensive biochemical studies Selleck EGFR inhibitor in vitro, the conformation of synuclein in cells remains poorly

understood. In contrast to the N-terminal membrane binding domain, the C terminus of human α-synuclein is polar, with a higher proportion of charged residues. This domain undergoes almost phosphorylation at multiple sites (Oueslati et al., 2010 and Sato et al., 2013), suggesting a mechanism for regulation, but the function of the C terminus remains unclear, and it is the least conserved domain across species as well as among α-, β-, and γ- isoforms. The C terminus may affect membrane binding under particular conditions (Shvadchak et al., 2011), but phosphorylation toward the end of the N-terminal repeats, at Ser-87, more clearly affects membrane binding in vitro than phosphorylation at the other, more C-terminal sites (Paleologou et al., 2010). The observations thus suggest a potential biological role for Ser-87 phosphorylation, although this again remains to be identified in the context of the cell. The presynaptic location of α-synuclein has been recognized since its original identification as a protein associated with synaptic vesicles (Maroteaux et al., 1988). In contrast to many proteins involved in neurodegeneration that are distributed throughout the neuron, however, α-synuclein localizes specifically to the nerve terminal, with relatively little in the cell body, dendrites, or extrasynaptic sites along the axon (George et al., 1995 and Iwai et al., 1995).

, 1991) From a cell biological perspective, the organization of

, 1991). From a cell biological perspective, the organization of dendrodendritic synapses raises the question of how specialized vesicle release and postsynaptic subdomains are established and maintained in close

proximity to each other in the same cell. Many fundamental questions regarding the formation and maintenance of these synapses remain unanswered (Figure 2). At presynaptic terminals, endocytosis is required for regenerating synaptic vesicles (Heuser and Reese, 1973 and Robitaille and Tremblay, 1987). Is there a similar specialization near dendrodendritic active zones? In hippocampal neurons, an endocytic zone adjacent to the PSD is required for retaining and recycling glutamate receptors at individual synapses, disruption of which results in a loss of synaptic glutamate receptors (Blanpied and Ehlers, 2004, Blanpied et al., 2002, Lu et al., 2007, Petrini et al., 2009 and Rácz et al., 2004). Perhaps a similar endocytic domain is selleck compound responsible for maintaining neurotransmitter receptors or regenerating dendritic vesicles at dendrodendritic synapses. If this is the case, how is segregation maintained between the recycling membrane pools containing neurotransmitter and the recycling membrane pools containing receptors? If the axon terminal and dendritic release machinery

are the same, how do mitral click here cells subvert the normal polarized trafficking itinerary of axonal molecules required for neurotransmitter release and redirect them to dendrites? Are there components that bridge postsynaptic densities and vesicle release sites that are required to maintain these subdomains in close but nonoverlapping proximity? What are the cell adhesion molecules that bridge the dendrodendritic synapse? The dendrodendritic synapse represents a remarkable exception to the normal

rules of neuronal cell polarity; thus the answers to these questions will provide insight not only into the details of olfactory circuitry, but also general principles of how cellular subdomains are specified and maintained. Dopamine has been observed in a variety of dendritic organelles including large dense core vesicles, small synaptic vesicles, and tubulovesicular structures resembling smooth ER in dopaminergic neurons (Björklund and Lindvall, 1975 and Nirenberg et al., 1996). Depletion of dendritic dopamine by reserpine treatment, a vesicular monamine isothipendyl transporter (VMAT) inhibitor, was the first evidence that dopamine in dendrites was a secreted, biologically active pool of neurotransmitter (Björklund and Lindvall, 1975 and Nirenberg et al., 1996). Perhaps the best characterized neurons that exhibit dendritic dopamine release are dopamine neurons of the substantia nigra (SN), which have long striatal axonal projections. The anatomical separation of dendrites and axon terminals in the nigrostriatal circuit makes it possible to measure dendritic dopamine release with little contamination from axonal release.

As in earlier

As in earlier this website studies, Voronoi tessellation was used to transform the discretely sampled surface into a continuous map using the assumption that each point on the map has the response characteristics of the nearest recording site (Kilgard and Merzenich, 1998). Because regions with above average sampling density have smaller tessellations, they do not bias estimates of the cortical response. A1 sites were identified on the basis of latency and topography. The percent of the cortical area of A1 responding to each tone was estimated as the sum of the

areas of all tessellations from sites in A1 with receptive fields that included the tone, divided by the total area of the field. For the time course study in which animals were mapped after NBS-tone pairing alone, we measured the percentage of A1 cortex that responded to the frequency that was paired with NBS, a 19 kHz, 60 Selleck CB-839 dB SPL tone. For all behaviorally trained animals, we reported changes in the representation of behaviorally relevant tones by reporting the ratio of the percent of cortex that responded to a 2 kHz, 60 dB SPL tone divided by the percentage of cortex that responded to a 19 kHz, 60 dB SPL tone. In behaviorally trained animals, we commonly observe both a shift in tuning toward behaviorally relevant tones and a decrease in receptive field sizes (Figure S1). The net effect of

this plasticity is to cause the cortical response to behaviorally irrelevant tones

to decrease whereas the response to behaviorally relevant tones is only slightly increased or unchanged (Figure S1). Therefore a ratio measure provides a reliable indicator of the relative frequency organization of low versus high tones in A1. Discrimination performance was measured using the signal detection Thymidine kinase theory measure d′ during all stages of training ( Abdi, 2010 and Klein, 2001). Statistical comparisons between three or more groups were done using repeated-measure ANOVA. Tones <0.38 octaves above the target stimulus were excluded from the repeated-measure ANOVA because these sounds were not reliably discriminated from the target stimulus and therefore were not expected to change significantly after NBS-tone pairing. Statistical comparisons between only two groups and single tone frequencies relative to zero were done using t tests, and t tests were used for all statistical comparisons of physiological measures between two groups. Unless otherwise noted, p-values reported are for two-tailed t tests. This work was supported by the James S. McDonnell Foundation. The authors would like to thank the many undergraduate volunteers who contributed to data collection and would like to thank Nick Reed, Crystal Engineer, Navzer Engineer, Kamalini Ranasinghe, Jai Shetake, and Benjamin Porter for comments on the manuscript.

, 2003), suggesting that exon 5 inclusion results in decreased de

, 2003), suggesting that exon 5 inclusion results in decreased dendritic localization. Moreover, Tanc proteins interact with PSD-95, a protein involved in localizing NMDARs at the synapse ( Han et al., 2010). Thus, a decrease in dendritic GRIN1 or altered regulation of NMDARs INCB018424 datasheet could underlie the decrease in the synaptic NMDAR response, impaired LTP, and learning

and memory deficits. While there have been sporadic reports of epilepsy associated with DM, this is not a characteristic overt feature of this disease (Meola and Sansone, 2007). However, DM patients show enhanced sensitivity to barbituates and benzodiazepines, which enhance the activity of the GABAA receptor (Harper, 2001). Moreover, the relevance of this hyperexcitability phenotype to DM1 is also supported by our observation that seizures were induced with a GABA antagonist in both Mbnl2 knockouts and the DMSXL transgenic model for DM1. Because the expression of a large CTG expansion in the DMSXL brain is sufficient to increase seizure susceptibility in mice, this study provides

the cautionary note that DM may possess some features of an excitability disorder. Finally, we also noted a difference in the latency time to the appearance of the seizure phenotype between males and females, which could reflect sex-specific differences in the alternative splicing of seizure-associated genes. Based on these and additional findings, we propose a modified MBNL combinatorial loss-of-function model for Antidiabetic Compound Library high throughput DM. MBNL proteins function as alternative splicing factors during postnatal development, with MBNL2 the predominant factor in the brain, while MBNL1 serves a similar role in skeletal muscle (Figure 7E). Thus, we anticipate that major pathological changes in the DM brain are attributable

to toxic RNA expression, MBNL2 sequestration by Cell press C(C)UGexp RNAs, and dysregulation of specific alternative splicing events required for normal adult CNS function. The Mbnl2 targeting vector was derived from CHORI clone bMQ-63F6. A 10 kb fragment containing Mbnl2 was subcloned into PL253 (protocols 1–4, http://web.ncifcrf.gov/research/brb/protocol.aspx). The targeting construct ( Figure S1B, Table S3 for PCR primers) was linearized with NotI and electroporated in 129 SvlmJ ES cells, followed by selection as described ( Kanadia et al., 2003). For constitutive Mbnl2 knockouts, Mbnl2+/con mice were mated to B6.C-Tg(CMV-cre)1Cgn/J mice (JAX). All animal procedures were approved by the University of Florida IACUC. Mouse tissue protein and RNA analyses ( Kanadia et al., 2006), immunohistochemistry and X-Gal staining ( Emamian et al., 2003), and rotarod analysis ( Daughters et al., 2009) were performed as described previously with some modifications (see Supplemental Experimental Procedures). Implant surgery, EEG/EMG monitoring, and EEG data acquisition were performed on 6-month-old mice (n = 8 for each genotype) as described (Fujiki et al., 2009).

34 ± 0 23, n = 7) We also examined the weighted decay time const

34 ± 0.23, n = 7). We also examined the weighted decay time constant (τW) of the NMDAR EPSCs recorded at +40 mV and found that it was larger in nigrostriatal neurons when compared to the other neuronal subpopulations although this difference reached statistical significance only when compared to the decay time constant of neurons projecting to mPFC (Figure S2A, mesolimbic lateral shell neurons:

75.0 ± 19.4 ms, DAPT in vivo n = 10; nigrostriatal neurons: 138.5 ± 16.5 ms, n = 9; mesocortical neurons: 52.5 ± 10.0 ms, n = 10; mesolimbic medial shell neurons: 88.5 ± 17.2 ms, n = 8). Finally, we measured paired-pulse ratios at 50 ms and 100 ms interstimulus intervals (Figure S2B) but found no differences between the subpopulations of neurons in this estimate of the average

probability of transmitter release. The larger AMPAR/NMDAR ratios in mesocortical and mesolimbic medial selleck chemicals shell neurons are consistent with our suggestion that these neurons have not previously been studied and suggest that the basal properties of their excitatory synapses are different from synapses on mesolimbic lateral shell neurons and nigrostriatal neurons. Given that some of the basic properties of DA neurons differ depending on the brain regions to which they project, a critical question is whether these neuronal subpopulations are all modulated in the same manner by a “rewarding” experience. To address this issue, we took advantage of the well-established modification of excitatory synapses on VTA DA neurons caused by in vivo administration of drugs of abuse, an increase in the AMPAR/NMDAR ratio (Ungless et al., 2001, Saal et al., 2003, Borgland et al., 2004, Dong et al., 2004, Faleiro et al., 2004, Liu et al., 2005, Bellone

and Lüscher, 2006, Argilli et al., 2008, Chen et al., 2008, Engblom et al., 2008 and Heikkinen et al., 2009). Twenty-four hours prior to slice preparation, cocaine (15 mg/kg, ip) or, in most experiments, saline (0.9%, ip, volume matched for experimental injections) was administered to animals mafosfamide that 1–3 weeks previously had been injected with Retrobeads. Consistent with previous results, neurons projecting to NAc lateral shell and which express a large Ih exhibited a clear increase in their AMPAR/NMDAR ratios after cocaine administration (Figure 3A: saline, 0.33 ± 0.06, n = 7; cocaine, 0.61 ± 0.05, n = 13; p = 0.003). Surprisingly, however, cocaine did not significantly increase AMPAR/NMDAR ratios in either nigrostriatal cells (Figure 3B, saline: 0.34 ± 0.02, n = 6; cocaine: 0.48 ± 0.06, n = 14; p = 0.169) or in VTA cells projecting to mPFC (Figure 3C, control: 0.61 ± 0.04, n = 10; cocaine: 0.59 ± 0.07, n = 6; p = 0.765). In contrast, even though the basal AMPAR/NMDAR ratios were high, a large increase occurred in VTA DA neurons projecting to NAc medial shell (Figure 3D, saline: 0.60 ± 0.07, n = 5; cocaine: 1.1 ± 0.08, n = 9; p = 0.002). Cocaine administration did not affect the paired-pulse ratios in any DA neuron subpopulations (data not shown).

The consequence is a sparse, efficient representation (mostly in<

The consequence is a sparse, efficient representation (mostly in

predictor neurons) of predictable input, and a robust, distributed response (mostly in error neurons) to unpredictable input, both coordinated across multiple levels of the processing hierarchy (Figure 1). Within a cortical region, population activity reflects a mixture of responses in the predictor neurons (passing information about predicted inputs down the hierarchy) and the error neurons (passing information about unpredicted inputs up the hierarchy). In principle, predictive coding models need make no assumption about the distribution of these two kinds of neurons within a population; in practice, aggregate population activity is often dominated by error neurons (Friston, 2009, Wacongne et al., 2012, Egner et al., 2010, Keller et al., selleck chemicals 2012 and Meyer and Sauerland, 2009). The result is that the

classic signature of predictive coding, reduced activity to predictable stimuli, is typically observed when averaging across large samples of neurons selleck chemicals llc within a region (Meyer and Olson, 2011, Egner et al., 2010 and de Gardelle et al., 2013). However, (as described in more detail below) signatures of the predictor neurons can also be observed; for example, the predictor neurons would likely show increased response when the input matches their predictions (e.g., de Gardelle et al., 2013). Following work in sensory processing (e.g., Wacongne et al., 2012), in our proposal both error neurons and predictor neurons convey “representational” information, and both are likely tuned to specific stimuli or stimulus features. Predictor neurons, present at each level of the cortical hierarchy, do not code a “complete” representation of the expected stimulus, but only some features or dimensions of the stimulus, at a relevant level of processing. Each set of predictor neurons can explain only those particular features or dimensions of the input, and correspondingly modulates the response in a highly specific subset of error neurons. Error neurons are similarly distributed throughout the cortex and respond to specific stimulus features (Meyer and Olson, 2011 and den Ouden et al., 2012),

rather Tryptophan synthase than, for example, a single “error region” signaling the overall amount of error or degree to which the observed stimulus is unpredicted (e.g., Hayden et al., 2011). Thus, for example, in the early visual cortex, predictor neurons code information about the predicted orientation and contrast at a certain point in the visual field, and error neurons signal mismatches between the observed orientation and contrast and the predicted orientation and contrast. In IT cortex, predictor neurons code information about object category; error neurons signal mismatches in predicted and observed object category (den Ouden et al., 2012 and Peelen and Kastner, 2011). One consequence of this model is that, typically, the effects of predictions are limited to relatively few levels of the processing hierarchy.

Similarly, the decreased synchrony between hippocampal and prefro

Similarly, the decreased synchrony between hippocampal and prefrontal neural activity at both the

oscillatory activity and single unit levels suggests an impairment in long-range functional connectivity between these structures. The hypothesis that long-range connections are impaired in schizophrenia is an idea that has been gaining traction, most recently with the advent of functional and structural imaging techniques capable of examining connectivity in the intact human brain (Pettersson-Yeo et al., 2011). A key challenge has been to relate such findings Selleckchem isocitrate dehydrogenase inhibitor to specific etiological causes—to chart out the possible courses on the map. Here, progress is being made in a variety of ways. Deficits in connectivity have been linked to specific genetic mutations through both animal (Sigurdsson et al., 2010) and human (Esslinger et al., 2009) studies, as well as to an animal model of an environmental risk factor (Dickerson et al., 2010). The current study extends these findings to demonstrate deficits in functional connectivity in a toxin-induce model of a neurodevelopmental deficit that, as noted DAPT above, shares several neuropathological, neurochemical, and behavioral features with schizophrenia. Establishing

the relevance of these results from the MAM-E17 rat model to the neurobiology of schizophrenia will require further work. Beyond clarifying the mechanistic details, one key next step will be to examine sleep and sleep-related oscillations in animal models based on specific, known etiological factors, such as the 22q11 microdeletion or maternal infection models. Demonstrating similar findings in etiologically-relevant models will provide convergent validity, helping fill in the map of potential explanatory pathways. In addition, studies in such models will help address a principle limitation of the MAM-E17 model, which, though it shares

important features with schizophrenia, does not mimic any known specific cause of the disease. In the end, however, the utility of a model is not a function of how many features it shares with a disease, but rather how well it can be exploited to generate and test hypotheses about how that Histone demethylase disease arises and manifests in the brain. The Phillips et al. (2012) study suggests that neurodevelopmental disturbances can give rise to disturbances in the structure and quantity of non-REM sleep. It inspires the novel hypothesis that such disturbances might lead to the impaired cognitive function observed in schizophrenia. And it suggests specific, testable hypotheses for circuit-based mechanisms that might generate these disturbances. “
“Functional MRI has established itself as a cornerstone method of modern neuroscience research.